My thanks to Tim Love for raising an important issue
about the relationship between literary criticism and literary fiction. Tim wonders if
theory influences student writers directly, or if it affects the zeitgeist,
making editors more likely to take on certain types of writing.
It’s a good question, especially now
that there are so many creative writing/MFA programs alongside literature
departments in American and British, universities, making the connection
between criticism and creative writing all the more possible. Kelcey Parker, in her essay (which I
mentioned last week) on the relationship between the rise of creative writing
programs and the concurrent rise of the “New Formalism,” discusses this issue,
and I will come back to it next week when I attempt an examination of links
between the “new formalism” and the “old formalism.”
However, as I think about the relationship between
what has been taught in university literature classrooms in the twentieth
century and what has been written by short story writers, at least in America,
I do see a general correlation, although I am not sure which came first—the
theory or the story. For example,
during the 1940s and 1950s, when Formalism or New Criticism was the dominant
academic approach to literature, the dominant short story form was the
so-called “traditional” or “well-made” story, e.g. the stories of John Cheever,
Eudora Welty, Peter Taylor, Katherine Anne Porter, Bernard Malamud, etc. When
structuralism and deconstruction were enjoying a brief heyday in the 1960’s,
the self-reflexive stories of William H. Gass, Donald Barthelme, John Barth,
and John Cheever interested academics. I am not sure what brand of criticism
can be correlated with the so-called minimalism of Raymond Carver, Tobias
Wolfe, Ann Beattie, etc. in the 1970s. But when cultural and ethnic criticism
became popular in the academy, a number of writers representing various
cultures also became popular, e.g. Sandra Cisneros, Jhumpa Lahiri, Junot Diaz,
Aleksandar Hemon. I will come back to
this question next week when I make a leap over culture criticism to the “new
formalism.” But, for now, indulge me
in an admittedly oversimplified summary of Structuralism, Deconstruction, and
Reader Response criticism.
Taking its initial cue from Russian Formalism, Structuralism began as a
reflection of the need to understand literary criticism as a unified scientific
field of study rather than the practice of the explication of individual works
of art. Concerned not with meaning but
more generally with what makes meaning itself possible, Structuralism gets its
most powerful and immediate impetus from the methods of modern linguistics as
developed primarily by Swiss linguist Ferdinand de Saussure. The seminal
document of modern Structuralism is Course in General Linguistics, a
collection of Saussure's lecture notes edited by some of his students,
originally published in 1915 but not translated into English until 1959.
The central ideas of Saussure that have proven most useful to literary
theory by the Structuralists are fairly easy to summarize, although their
implications have proven highly complex and controversial. Saussure's basic assertion was that
"language" should not be thought of simply as a horde of those words
we use with which to communicate.
Instead, language is made up both of individual utterances (which
Saussure called parole) as well as the general system of language that
makes such individual utterances possible (which Saussure called langue).
Although individual utterances make up the governing system of language, they
do so not as an aggregate of utterances, but rather as an elaborate system of
generative principles.
Furthermore, the individual "sign," such as designated by a
single word in a language, is also made up of two parts. First, there is the sound that we make when
we utter the word "house"; then there is the concept we have in our
minds when we utter such a word. The
sound image Saussure calls the "signifier," whereas he calls the
mental concept the "signified."
What is important to remember about these two notions is that there is
no intrinsic or "necessary" relationship between the two. There is no essential quality of
"houseness" inherent in the sound we make when we say
"house." The relationship
between the two is purely arbitrary and conventional; it results from the tacit
agreement of those who belong to a certain speech community that such a sound
image will signify such a concept.
The final distinction Saussure made that has become important to the
study of literature is the distinction between studying a phenomenon, such as
language or literature, as it develops over time, (which Saussure calls a
"diachronic" study) and studying it as it exists at any one given
moment in time (which Saussure calls a "synchronic" study). These two approaches for studying a cultural
phenomenon are related to the realization that all utterances and other
examples of sign systems communicate simultaneously in two different ways: first
along the linear, time-bound axis, as the sentence, "the dog bites the
boy" communicates by the syntactic relationship between the signifiers
"dog," "bites," and "boy"; and second along a
vertical, spatially-fixed axis that exists tacitly for each of the signifiers
in the string on the basis of similarity of function. For example, the signifier "dog" could be replaced by
"cat," "snake," "lion," in short anything that
might plausibly perform the same function as the signifier
"dog." The linear
relationship between the signifiers in the string is called the
"syntagmatic" relationship, whereas the spatial relationship is
called the "paradigmatic" relationship. The first is governed by the principle of contiguity or
combination of signifiers, whereas the second is governed by the principle of
similarity or substitution.
In Fundamentals of Language (1969), Roman Jakobson suggests that
the distinction between the syntagmatic and the paradigmatic corresponds not
only to the two basic ways that simple linguistic chains signify, but also two
basic means by which larger units of linguistic chains such as literary works
signify. All discourse, says Jakobson,
communicates along two lines of meaning: one topic may lead to another through
the process of combination based on contiguity, or else a topic may lead to
another through the process of substitution based on similarity. The first corresponds to the trope known as
"metonymy," whereby something is suggested by something else
contiguous to it; for example a doghouse may "stand for" a dog
because a dog lives there. The second
corresponds to the device known as "metaphor," whereby something is
suggested by something else that can be substituted for it; for example a dog
can be referred to as an animal, a pet, man's best friend, a pest, etc.
The first significant attempt to use the linguistic approach to apply
to a signifying phenomenon other than language itself was the effort by French
anthropologist Claude Levi-Strauss to understand myth. In his most familiar
discussion, "The Structural Study of Myth," a chapter from his 1958
book Structural Anthropology, Levi-Strauss laid out an approach to myth
that has since been used for the study of literary fictions.
Myth is story, says Levi-Strauss, made up of basic constituent units or
distinctive features that share similar functional traits. However, these units are larger than the
units of phonemes or morphemes that make up language; thus Levi-Strauss calls
them "gross constituent units," and terms them "mythemes." After breaking down the myth into mythemes,
or units based on similarity of function, Levi-Strauss then determines how
these units are related to each other in what he calls "bundles of
relations." He then reads the myth
not in terms of one event after another in a causal-chronological relationship,
but rather in terms of the logical relationships between the various sets or
"bundles."
In this way, Levi-Strauss breaks down the syntagmatic flow of the myth
based on contiguity, groups the resulting motifs together into paradigmatic
sets based on similarity, and then reads the paradigmatic sets in terms of
their logical relationships. The result
is, as Roman Jakobson stated in his famous 1958 "Closing Statement:
Linguistics and Poetics," similarity is imposed onto contiguity and thus
equivalence is made the constitutive device of the sequence. In other words,
the syntagmatic, which is "just one thing after another" and
therefore meaningless, is transformed into paradigmatic sets made up of units
based on similarity which communicate by logical relationships. Levi-Strauss's method has served as the
model for further studies of literary narratives as if they were structured the
same way language is.
Structuralist critics have been primarily concerned with various ways
to extrapolate from the study of language a method for the study of
literature. The most basic way they
attempted to do this in the 1960's was to treat literature as a second-level
language system above language itself.
Although a poem or a story is made up of language and thus can be broken
down into such units as phonemes and morphemes, Structuralists made use of
Saussure's ideas of the distinction between langue and parole to
refer to a distinction within literature itself between the individual work of
art (parole) and the system of genre (langue) to which it
belonged, or else between the genre (parole) and the larger system of
literature as a whole (langue). Carrying this approach even further,
they suggested that literature was not only made up of language, it was
"like" a language in many other ways as well. The field of study that has made the most
extensive use of such linguistic approaches is the field that Structuralism may
be said to have invented--narratology.
Drawing their inspiration initially from Vladimir Propp's Morphology
of the Folktale, such narratologists as A. J. Greimas, Claude Bremond, and
Tzvetan Todorov were concerned with identifying the fundamental elements of
narrative and their laws of combination.
Perhaps the most familiar to Anglo-American readers is Todorov, whose
collection The Poetics of Prose, translated in 1977, made his approach
easily accessible. Basically, Todorov
reduces the action in individual stories to a basic syntactic summary and then
analyzes that summary by focusing on active verb forms in the stories such as
"to change," "to transgress," "to punish,"
etc. It has often been pointed out that
whereas such an approach works most effectively with highly formalized works
such as stories and tales, it works least effectively with more
"realistic" works such as the novel.
The Structuralist approach to narrative, claim many of its critics,
drains the human content out of literary works and then deals only with their
mathematically pure, linguistic-like, structure.
Reader-Response
Theory
Because of this refusal to deal with the human origin of the work, its
human content, or its human effect on the reader, the Structuralist approach
was challenged almost as soon as it began by critics concerned with the
"subjectivity" of literature, particularly with the subjective
involvement or response of the reader.
However, there are two distinct sources for literary criticism that
focuses on the reader--the phenomenological theory derived from Edmund Husserl
and the psychoanalytic theory derived from Sigmund Freud. The first has often been called
"Reception Aesthetics," whereas the second has been termed
"Transactive Criticism."
The Phenomenologists criticize such linguistically-based approaches as
Structuralism because they try to fix invariant patterns in literary works and
thus abstract the human being out of the work's concrete experience. The subtlest spokesman of Phenomenology's
interest in understanding the subjectivity of literature from the inside rather
than objectively from the outside is philosopher Paul Ricoeur. However, Ricoeur's discussions of how
meaning is created in The Rule of Metaphor (1977) and how history is
like narrative in Time and Narrative (1984) have had less effect on
literary criticism than European Reception Theory introduced in German in the
late 1960's by Hans Robert Jauss.
Making use of Husserl's basic notion that one perceives reality through
an abstract structure of expectation (termed "horizons"), Jauss
argues that to study literary history, the focus should be on the reader's
literary horizons, that is, the structure of generic norms the reader has
internalized as a result of all previous texts he or she has read. Following this same approach, Wolfgang Iser,
the best-known spokesman for Reception Theory now in the United States, focuses
on reading as a dynamic process during which the reader continually fills in
what Iser calls "gaps of indeterminacy" in the text--gaps which are
there because the art work never completely corresponds to real objects. Iser's reader is not one who brings his or
her unique experience to the reading experience, but rather is what Iser
defines as an "implicit reader," one who alters the self to fit the
kind of reader that the work requires.
This simply means that the reading experience is a dynamic interchange
with the text, not a passive experience; the person you are for the time you
are reading Huckleberry Finn, for example, is not quite the same person
you are when you read The Scarlet Letter.
Another well-known advocate of reader-based criticism is American
critic Stanley Fish, who has called his approach to literary texts
"Affective Stylistics." Like
Iser, Fish's notion of a reader is not one who brings to the text all the
individualities that define him or her in everyday life, but rather is what
some have called a "superreader” who interacts with the text in a
highly-sophisticated rhetorical way.
Fish says that the so-called "objectivity" of a text is a
dangerous illusion; reading, and thus the text itself, constitute a temporal, not
a spatial, experience, as Formalists, Myth Critics, and Structuralists say that
it is. A sentence, for example, argues
Fish, is not an object, but an event, something that happens to, and with the
participation of, the reader. Fish says
that in his method of analyzing a work, he monitors the temporal flow of the
experience as it is structured by what the reader brings with him or her, and
thus he can chart reader response as one that develops in time.
In contrast to Phenomenologically-based Reader Response Theory, which
focuses primarily on the reader's general and rhetorical expectations as he or
she reads the work, psychoanalytically-based Reader-Response Theory focuses on
the reader's specific response based on his or her unique personality or identity. The best-known advocate of this brand of
criticism, sometimes called
"Transactive Criticism" or "Buffalo Criticism,"
because it originates from State University of New York, Buffalo, is Norman
Holland.
Holland's first major theoretical book, The Dynamics of Literary
Response (1968) came at a time when new Criticism's explication of
individual poems was beginning to pale on critics and students and when much
psychoanalytic criticism up to that point, influenced both by Formalism and
Myth Criticism, had degenerated into the simple interpretative task of
searching for dream or myth symbols in literary works. Holland argued that
Freud's theories, particularly in his study of wit and jokes, offered the basis
for a general theory about the dynamic transaction between reader and text in
which basic interests or themes in the reader's personality
"constructed" themes in the text.
Thus, like Fish, Holland urges that texts should not be studied as
objects but rather as dynamic transactions between readers and texts. Holland
argues that by means of literary form, (which works like defense mechanisms in
human beings) and by means of literary meaning (which works the way sublimation
does in human beings), literature can transform unconscious desires in the reader
into a higher aesthetic, intellectual, and moral unity. This unity, which exists not just in the
text, but which is created by the needs of the reader, is what the critic
should focus on.
The second most familiar critic within this psychoanalytic-based reader-response
tradition is David Bleich, whose first book, Readings and Feelings: An
Introduction to Subjective Criticism (1975) had a major impact on the way
literature is taught in the classroom, particularly the high school classroom,
in the United States. His more
substantial theoretical book, Subjective Criticism (1978), criticizes
Holland for focusing too much on the objectivity of the text, as the New
Critics did, and instead offers a radical new "subjective" paradigm
of thought which is based on epistemological issues of how one
"knows"; consequently, he connects psychoanalytically-based Reader-
Response Theory with some of the issues that have dominated
phenomenologically-based theories about the reader.
Deconstruction
It is an interesting irony of modern criticism that even as
Structuralism was being introduced to American critics in 1966 at a conference
at Johns Hopkins University entitled "The Languages of Criticism and the
Sciences of Man," a relatively unknown philosopher named Jacques Derrida
delivered a paper entitled "Structure, Sign, and Play in the Discourse of
the Human Sciences" which was already seriously challenging
Structuralism. In this milestone essay,
Derrida challenged the basic assumptions of Structuralism as illustrated by Claude
Levi-Strauss; then in a series of important, but often dense and unreadable
books published in the following year, On Grammatology, Writing and
Difference, and Speech and Phenomena, he further undercut
Structuralism's philosophic foundations as established by Ferdinand de
Saussure. His approach, which has come to be called "deconstruction,"
is to analyze such thinkers as Levi-Strauss, Saussure, and Edmund Husserl in
such a way as to show that their own arguments undermine themselves and thus
create a basic contradiction which itself is the key to understanding.
As a result of Derrida's critique of Structuralism, the movement never
really got started in America.
Moreover, by the time it was introduced to English-speaking critics, its
most influential advocate in Europe, Roland Barthes, had already begun to offer
his own challenging critique primarily in his work S/Z (1970). Whereas in his earlier critical statements
Barthes, like other Structuralists, had appealed to a general structure,
something equivalent to Saussure's notion of langue from which one could
derive an analysis of an individual text or parole; in S/Z
Barthes analyzed a short novel by Balzac as being a work which instead of
having a single parole-like system governed by its dependence on a large
langue-like system, is a system in and of itself. Barthes argued that there is no transcendent
or primary model equivalent to langue, but rather that each text is
traversed by numerous codes which constitute its meaning. The implication of this shift is that if
that the text does not have a meaning determined by a transcendent code it may
have numerous meanings which are created by the reader as he or she applies the
various procedures demanded by the multiple codes that traverse it.
However, it is Derrida's challenge to Structuralism's assumptions of a
transcendent code that has had the most powerful impact on contemporary
literary theory. In his 1966
presentation at the Johns Hopkins Conference on Structuralism, Derrida
challenged the methodology of Levi-Strauss on the basis of what he called
Levi-Strauss's tacit nostalgia for a central and transcendent
"presence" or "fixed origin." Derrida exposes the Kantian basis of Structuralism and dismisses
as a fiction, albeit a functional fiction, the apriori mythic
consciousness on which all forms of Formalist criticism, from the Russian
Formalists to the Structuralists, had depended.
In referring to Saussure's influential distinction between signifier
and signified, Derrida argued there was no transcendent signified to which a
signifier referred, but that a signifier referred only to other signifiers in
an endless play of signifiers. Derrida
insisted that the Structuralist endeavor was based on what he called a
"metaphysics of presence," that is, some hypothetical mythic moment
when signifier and signified were intrinsically related and indivisible.
Derrida has claimed that this illusion is damaging, for it allows us to avoid
dealing with the reality of our fragmentary reality on the assumption that
there is some unified, pure meaning or reality that can be grasped. According to Derrida, everything is a
mediated "text"; there is nothing outside of the text, and all that
texts can refer to are other texts.
In a related move, Derrida dismissed the assumption of linguistics that
writing was secondary to and derivative of oral speech, for this, he said, was
just another version of the "metaphysics of presence." To believe that writing is secondary to
speech is to believe that although writing is a highly-mediated sign system
that one must interpret, its source is in speech, which, by comparison is
unmediated, and thus its truth is immediately knowable. Derrida argues, however, it is an illusion
to think that truth is apparent at the moment of speech. In fact, once it is shown that speech is
susceptible to the same distance and difference from meaning as writing itself
is and thus not a primary source of truth, then writing can be studied as the
model of what Derrida calls a "metaphysics of absence," which allows
for the "free play of signifiers."
According to Derrida, there has never been an original source; there has
never been anything but a string of substitutional signifiers in a chain of
differences on to infinity. The most
basic implication of Derrida's approach for literary criticism is that if a
work can have no ultimate meaning it can have limitless meanings.
It is this basic implication that American followers of Derrida,
primarily the so-called "Yale School," which includes Geoffrey Hartman,
J. Hillis Miller, Paul De Man, and Harold Bloom, most took to heart. Although these critics differ in many
particulars in terms of their engagement with the ideas of Derrida, basically
they all proceed on the assumption that the notion of referentiality is an
illusion. A sign, says J. Hillis
Miller, marks not the presence of, but the absence of, an object. All the world is a text in which there are
not facts, only interpretations.
Similarly Paul De Man argues that what reading reveals is the confrontation
with a language that always vacillates between the promise of some referential
meaning and the rhetorical subversion of that meaning. Only Harold Bloom differs in his approach by
focusing on the problem of literary history from a psychoanalytic point of
view. Although he agrees that every
text is an intertext, he argues that literary history is the history of the
clash of the strong personalities of young poets in conflict with powerful
previous poets or precursors.
Literature develops by means of purposeful "misreadings" by
present poets of previous ones.
Structuralist and Deconstruction theories about the nature of
literature have also been integrated into two of the most pervasive and
powerful models for the analysis of human experience in Western
culture--Marxism and Psychoanalysis. Although both of these models were guilty
of reductionism when first used by literary critics in the early part of the
20th century, more recent approaches to Marxism, derived primarily from the
so-called Frankfurt School of social theorists, and more recent explorations in
psychoanalysis, derived from the work of French analyst Jacques Lacan, have
attempted to make use of the linguistic revolution to better understand Marx's
critique of society and Freud's creation of the unconscious.
The best-known Marxist theorists of the Frankfurt School are Theodor
Adorno, its chief aesthetician, who argues that the greatness of the art work
is that it allows those things to be heard which ideology conceals; and Louis
Althusser, who urges that critics lay bare the author's
"problematic," that is, the unconscious infrastructure or base of his
"potential thoughts" which make up the existing "ideological
field" within which he works.
Pierre Macherey makes the connection between Marxism and Structuralist
and Post-Structuralist theories even more obvious in A Theory of Literary
Production, originally published in Paris in 1966 and translated into
English in 1978.
For Macherey, criticism is not explication, nor is literature mimetic. Criticism is a form of knowledge; its object
is not the literary work, but rather a product of literary criticism
itself. Whatever phenomenal reality is
revealed by the literary work has no prior existence but is rather the product
of the laws of the work's production; the task of criticism is to reveal these
laws. In an effort to connect the
concept of "ideology" with linguistic theories about structure,
Macherey argues that ideology cannot be reduced to a set of concepts; ideology
is, in fact, the tacit internalized realm of structures itself. It is this realm of the unsaid and the
unsayable that makes the said possible.
For Macherey, the task of criticism is not to try to articulate the
unsaid, the so-called latent meaning, but rather to lay bare the laws of the
production of the said.
French psychoanalyst Jacques Lacan, strongly influenced in his reading
of Freud by Saussure and Levi-Strauss, argues that the unconscious is
structured like a language and therefore needs to be understood linguistically. However, he begins to sound more
deconstructive than structuralist in his approach when he argues the signifier
is privileged over the signified and that the child's early ego development is
based on an illusion of wholeness and totality which obscures the reality of
one's fragmentary self. Rivaling
Derrida in the complexity and density of his ideas and his prose style, Lacan's
theories, which have been termed "French Freud," have had a profound
influence on psychoanalytic approaches to criticism in America. The journal Yale French Studies has
been most instrumental in disseminating the views of Lacan primarily in the
writings of such critics as Shoshana Felman, Peter Brooks, and Barbara Johnson,
who have offered new Lacanian psychoanalytical approaches to Henry James,
Charles Dickens, and Edgar Allan Poe.
Throughout the 1970's, the American Deconstruction critics, Hartman,
DeMan, Bloom, and Miller carried on a vigorous defense of deconstruction
against more traditional critics, primarily in the most important journals of
modern literary theory, such as Critical Inquiry, New Literary
History, and Diacritics. As might be expected, traditional critics
have accused deconstructionists of being subjective, relative, unreadable, and
perversely contradictory. And indeed,
if one follows Derrida's line of thought and rejects any ultimately absolute
meaning, then literary analysis becomes justified not on the basis of its
truth-value but rather on the basis of whether it is interesting. Deconstruction critics do not strive for
some final reading of a work, but rather attempt to present an engagement with
the work that indeed rivals the work itself for its fictionality and
imaginative structure.
It is this ultimate divorce, not only from historical and social
referentiality but also from any responsibility either to the world or to the
text, that has lead to a reaction against Deconstruction in the 1980's that
marked a return, although a return with a crucial difference, to the view of
literary works as cultural documents of political and historical significance—a
reaction to which gave rise to the Formalist or New Critical revolution the
1940s and 1950s in the first place.
2 comments:
Thoroughly helpful, especially since I've been struggling with the concept of referential vs. non-referential models of language lately.
Thank you very much. I'm currently working on a paper for my literary theory class and your posts (this one in particular) have helped me better understand Toril Moi's "Sexual/Textual Politics."
Kate Prudchenko
Twitter: @kprudchenko
Literarysteppenwolf.posterous.com
Post a Comment