While working on a keynote presentation I am scheduled to make at the
Alice Munro Symposium next month in Ottawa (My colleague Robert Thacker will
also give a keynote, and a number of Munro experts and other Canadian scholars
will make presentations), I have been
reading Keith Oatley's Such Stuff as
Dreams and Lisa Zunshine's Why We
Read Fiction, as well as a thick wad of research reports by cognitive
psychologists from Canada and the U.S. on so-called Theory of Mind.
If you are not familiar with the concept, Theory of Mind (sometimes
unfortunately shortened to ToM) is not really a "theory" of mind, but
rather refers to the ability humans have to formulate theories about the minds
of others. It seems to be a human
characteristic developed by children sometime during the third year of life.
Before the development of this ability, children do not know that other people
have minds. We always knew, didn't we, that all little children are egomaniacs.
The classic experiment involves children watching puppets in a scene in
which one puppet puts a toy in a red box and then leaves the room. Another puppet removes the toy and puts it in
a blue box. When the first puppet comes
back in, the children watching the scene are asked to predict where the puppet
will look for the object. Before the age
of four, children predict that the puppet will look in the blue box, for that
is where they know the toy is and thus where they assume the puppet thinks it
is also. After the age of four, the
children predict that the puppet will look in the red box, for they know that the
puppet doesn't know that the object has been moved. In other words, they now
have the ability to formulate a theory that the puppet has a mind. I apologize
to the cognitive psychologists who have formulated this if I have
oversimplified or misrepresented.
A recent study that received quite a bit of popular press in the U.S. was
reported by psychologists at the New School for Social Research in the Oct,
2013 issue of Science. I found the report at www.sciencemag.org under the title
"Reading Literary Fiction Improves Theory of Mind"; the researchers are David Comer Kidd and
Emanuele Castano. With a little extra research on the Internet, I was able to
find the Supplementary material for the report, which detailed the methodology
and the materials used. I was
particularly interested in the fact that one of the "literary"
stories used in the study was Alice Munro's "Corrie," on which I have
posted, and which has received quite a bit of response on this blog.
I also discovered that the "popular" fiction the researchers
used to juxtapose against "Corrie" was a romance story by Rosamunde
Pilcher entitled "Lalla," from her collection Love Stories. (Other literary stories are from the 2012 PEN/O.
Henry Award Stories; other popular stories are from Popular Fiction: An Anthology. Ed. Gary Hoppenstand. In the literary vs. popular test, in addition
to "Corrie," they used Sam Ruddick's "Leak" and Wendell
Berry's "Nothing Living Lives Alone."
For the popular, they used "Space Jockey" by Robert Heinlein,
"Too Many Have Lived" by Dashiell Hammett, as well as
"Lalla" Rosamunde Pilcher.
I will talk a bit about "Corrie" and Theory of Mind in my
presentation at Ottawa next month, and so will not duplicate that discussion
here. However, I do want to make some
suggestions about how the notion of Theory of Mind may be related to the thesis
I try to develop in the first chapter of my book I Am Your Brother: Short Story Studies. (I posted that first chapter a few months ago
on this blog if you are interested in looking at it.) Basically, I suggest that
Frank O'Connor's concept in his book The
Lonely Voice about the short story's focus on human isolation and
loneliness is the primordial story that constitutes human beings
existentially--their basic sense of aloneness and their yearning for union.
It seems to me
that the Theory of Mind hypothesis that before the age of four the child has no
notion that others have minds is related to Jean Piaget's theory that the young baby itself
constitutes its sole reality because the baby's universe contains no permanent
object, no “It” and therefore no “I” except the total and unconscious
egocentric self. Piaget tells us that the baby's objectless universe is made up
of "shifting and unsubstantial `tableaux’ that appear and are then totally
reabsorbed." However, during the second year of life, a kind of
"Copernican revolution" takes place in the child, a general
decentering process during which the child begins to perceive the self as an
object in a universe made up of permanent objects, a universe in which
causality is localized in space and objectified in things. Although Piaget's theory has been questioned
in recent years, I like its explanatory power.
In my book, I correlate Piaget's theory
with the concepts of Martin Buber, who identifies the "separation of the human body, as the
bearer of its perceptions, from the world round about it... Whenever the
sentence “I see the tree” is so uttered that it no longer tells of a relation
between the man and the tree, but establishes the perception of the tree as
object by the human consciousness, the barrier between subject and object has
been set up. The primary word I-It, the word of separation, has been spoken."
This fall from at-oneness, dramatized in
the Genesis story of the Fall and the story of Cain and Abel, is, it seems to
me, related to the discovery of Theory of Mind children experience after their
third year—that there are other minds out there that can never be known.
Maybe
it is just because I am so committed to the underrated short story that I am
always trying to make a case for its importance, but it seems to me that the
story that is short—which is a primary narrative form, not a derivative
narrative form like the novel—came into being as the primary means by which
human beings, confronting the realization that they can never know the mind of
the other and thus are forever trapped within their solipsistic isolation, try
to identify with the other. Frank
O'Connor uses one of the earliest "modern" short stories, Gogol's
"The Overcoat," as an example of the thematizing of this notion,
which is why I use the plaintive cry Akakey Akakievich makes to his colleague—"I
Am Your Brother"—as the title of my most recent book.
H. Porter Abbot, my colleague at the
University of California, Santa Barbara, who uses Bartleby as an example,
focuses on fictional minds that cannot be read, not only by characters in the
story world but also by readers in the actual world. When my students
encountered a character whose behavior just did not make sense to them, for
example Bartleby's stubborn preference "not to", Kurtz's journey into
the heart of darkness, Gatsby's passion for the silly Daisy—they simply said,
"they must be crazy." As
Ginger Nut, like my students, puts it, "he's a little luny." This, of course, is the easy evasion of the
challenge to know the other that seems mysteriously unknowable.
I am not sure it is mere coincidence
that the American writer often credited with first recognizing the unique
characteristics of the short story, Edgar Allan Poe, was concerned with the
mystery of motivation and the efforts human beings make to try to project
themselves into the mind of the other. Poe's
stories about the mysteries of what makes people do the strange, contrary
things they do, focus on what he calls "the perverse." "The Imp of the Perverse" begins in
an essay format describing a human propensity—what the author calls a "prima mobilia of the human
soul"--previously ignored because there seems to be no reason for it,
either in a divine or a human plan.
The writer argues that if one proceeded à posteriori from observable evidence, rather than à priori from previous assumptions about God's plan, thinkers would have to admit "an innate and primitive principle of human action, a paradoxical something, which we may call perverseness, for want of a more characteristic term." He then describes the principle as a "mobile without motive, a motive not motivirit." However, more than acting without a comprehensible object or motive, the principle involves acting precisely because one should not. This radical impulse fascinates Poe because it cannot be broken down any further; it evades analysis by its very elementary nature. He explores the concept further in "The Black Cat" and "The Tell-Tale Heart."
The writer argues that if one proceeded à posteriori from observable evidence, rather than à priori from previous assumptions about God's plan, thinkers would have to admit "an innate and primitive principle of human action, a paradoxical something, which we may call perverseness, for want of a more characteristic term." He then describes the principle as a "mobile without motive, a motive not motivirit." However, more than acting without a comprehensible object or motive, the principle involves acting precisely because one should not. This radical impulse fascinates Poe because it cannot be broken down any further; it evades analysis by its very elementary nature. He explores the concept further in "The Black Cat" and "The Tell-Tale Heart."
The effort to project the self into
the mind of the other can best be seen in "The Purloined Letter," in
which Poe's detective Dupin gives an example of the school boy who is a master
at playing the game of "even and odd"--a guessing game in which one
holds a number of marbles in his hand and asks someone to guess whether they
are even or odd in number. The boy
succeeds in guessing by projecting himself into the one holding the marbles,
identifying with the opponent's intellect.
He says he fashions the expression of his face in accordance with the
face of the opponent and then waits to see what thoughts come to him.
In Such Stuff as Dreams, Keith Oatley cites Arthur Conan Doyle's
Sherlock Holmes's use of the same technique: "I put myself in the man's
place and, having first gauged his intelligence, I try to imagine how I should
myself have proceeded under the same circumstances." Oatley also cites G. K. Chesterton's Father Brown, who says: "When I tried to imagine the state of mind in which
such a thing would be done, I always realized that I might have done it myself
under certain mental conditions, but not under others."
I suppose I could be accused of
being obsessed with this basic theme of the short story, for I have been
exploring it since my first published article, a piece entitled "The
Difficulty of Loving in Eudora Welty's 'A Visit of Charity'." Maybe so.
But this new research on Theory of Mind intrigues me. Perhaps I can use it to further make my case
that the short story is a very importance literary form—a form that Jorge Luis
Borges once referred to as "essential."
I am especially interested in this
research that seems to argue for the importance of fiction in light of the fact
that most states in the U.S. are now adopting the so-called "Common Core,"
which has reduced the importance of reading fiction in favor of requiring
students read more nonfiction.
More
about this later. And more about Alice
Munro when I get back from Canada.
1 comment:
Aha! I just took a (very short, very introductory) MOOC (online course) on cognitive poetics, primarily the Theory of Mind and its application to literature; I'd never heard of this before. Wonderful stuff, I'm looking forward to seeing your future comments.
(Karen Carlson)
Post a Comment